History Module: The Growth of New Neurons
in the Adult Human Brain Scientific knowledge at any given point
in time can be described as the particular set of theories and models to which
the scientific community currently subscribes to explain the world. What makes
science so interesting is that periods of consensus, when a particular scientific
paradigm predominates, are typically followed by periods of radical questioning,
in which the previously received wisdom gets shaken to its foundations.
One example of such a revolution occurred in the neurosciences in the early 1990s.
For over a century, scientists had agreed that in adult mammals, if nerve cells
in the brain were damaged or died, unlike other cells in the body, they were not
replaced. In other words, it was taken as an article of faith that new neurons
never developed in the adult human brain. Each of us was born with as many neurons
in our brains as we would ever have, and for the rest of our lives, the number
of these neurons decreased continuously, though they could continue making new
connections with one another until the day we died. But for the past
decade or so, this formerly unquestioned assumption has been gradually undermined,
and recent studies indicate that certain parts of the brains of primates, including
humans, may maintain their ability to produce new neurons throughout adult life. Source:
Barry L. Jacobs, Henriette van Praag, Fred H. Gage When
we speak of the “birth” of new neurons,
or neurogenesis, in the adult brain, what we are actually talking about is the
process by which stem cells, such as those found in the hippocampus, divide in
two. One of the two cells will remain a stem cell, while the other will turn into
a neuron. Through this process, which occurs throughout the body, cells
that could potentially do anything acquire their final specialization: some become
blood cells, while others become muscle or skin cells. And some others, as we
now know, become functional nerve cells, with dendrites and axons that connect
them to other parts of the hippocampus. The
following paragraphs review the events that led scientists to conclude that functional
new neurons very likely do continue to form in the adult human brain, a process
known as neurogenesis. As we shall see, a scientific revolution such as this one
doesn’t usually happen overnight. More often, a body of observations accumulates
gradually over a long time until it eventually refutes the original concept.
The scientific dogma that no new neurons form in the adult human brain resulted
in part from experiments conducted on rhesus monkeys in the 1960s
(in particular by Pasko Rakic of Yale University). These experiments focused on
the development of synapses to explain the plasticity of the brain. The
first set of results that began to contradict this dogma were published in 1965.
Working with adult rats, Joseph Altman and Gopal Das of the Massachusetts Institute
of Technology (MIT) reported the birth of new cells in fairly primitive regions
of the brain: the olfactory system and the dentate gyrus of the hippocampus. But
these scientists were still uncertain whether these were neurons or glial cells.
In the 1980s, other researchers detected the appearance
of new neurons in the brains of canaries that were learning new songs. Evidence
began to accumulate in other experiments as well, such as an increase in the brain
weight of rats after they had been trained to run labyrinths. But until the 1990s,
most scientists continued to endorse the position that no new neurons ever form
in the adult brain.
In
the late 1980s, Elizabeth Gould, who was studying the effects
of various hormones on the brain, was puzzled to observe several signs of the
birth of new neurons in the rat hippocampus. She mentioned these observations
in articles published in 1992 and 1993, but
they drew little attention from the scientific community. In 1996,
Dr. Gould and Bruce McEwen observed that stress reduced the formation of these
new neurons in the brains of rats. These observations drew more attention, because
they appeared to show that the environment could influence the formation of nerve
cells.
In
spring 1998, Gould and her team reported having observed neurogenesis
in the hippocampus and the olfactory bulbs of adult primates (marmoset monkeys).
The following fall, the same observation was made in rhesus monkeys, which are
even more closely related to humans. These results in rhesus monkeys were soon
confirmed, in May 1999, by David R. Kornack and Pasko Rakic.
In
November 1998, the team of Swedish researcher Peter Eriksson
and American researcher Fred H. Gage published a study demonstrating that new
neurons are generated in the dentate gyrus of the adult human brain. The study
also showed that these new neurons were produced from stem cells, then migrated
through the granular layer and differentiated into mature neurons with axons and
dendrites. In March 1999, Gould’s team further refined
its observations by showing that the extent of neurogenesis was proportional to
the amount of cognitive stimulus that the animal received from its environment.
Thus, an enriched environment would promote neurogenesis while an impoverished
environment would reduce it. Likewise, rats that interacted socially were found
to have more new neurons than rats that were isolated in individual cages. In October 1999, Dr. Gould published an article, in
the journal Science, stating that new neurons were produced in three
associative regions of the neocortex of adult rhesus monkeys: the prefrontal,
inferior temporal, and posterior parietal cortexes. These regions are involved
in higher cognitive functions such as decisionmaking, short-term memory, recognition
of faces and the position of objects in space, and so on. Many scientists found
it far harder hard to accept the idea that even this most recently evolved part
of the brain could generate new neurons. In 2000, Eriksson and Gage published another study,
in which they showed that new neurons are generated in the dentate gyrus of people
up to 72 years of age. Also in 2000, other articles appeared that linked
neurogenesis in the hippocampus with depression. Researchers already knew that
stress slowed down neurogenesis. Now they observed that therapeutic interventions
that increased the level of serotonin in the brain also increased neurogenesis.
The idea that neurogenesis might play a role in precipitating and curing episodes
of depression began to be discussed more and more. In December 2001, Rakic published an article in which
he contradicted Gould’s results and concluded that though neurogenesis does
in fact occur in the hippocampus of adult primates, there is no compelling evidence
that it does so in their neocortex. The debate on this issue continues. In February 2002, Henriette van Praag, Fred H. Gage,
and their team reported that the membranes of the new neurons generated in the
hippocampus of adult mice have normal properties that enable them to propagate
action potentials and form functional synapses. The finding that new neurons can
develop in various parts of the brain and establish functional connections with
the neurons that were already there opens new prospects for the treatment of degenerative
diseases such as Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s.
Source: Heather A. Cameron
One-week-old neurons newly developed in the dentate gyrus of an adult rat brain
The
story continues… This story of how researchers determined
that new neurons do form in the brains of adult primates shows how most scientific
“discoveries” are made. The process
involves not so much the sudden revelation of a clear-cut fact as the long, gradual
accumulation of observations that contradict the initial state of knowledge. These
“anomalous” observations generate
new hypotheses (for example, that neurogenesis may occur in some other, more highly
evolved species, or in some other part of the brain), which lead to new experiments.
After a time, scientists put the newly observed facts together and formulate
a new theory. In the case described above, the new theory was that, contrary to
what was then believed, in certain regions of the primate brain, neurogenesis
does not stop when the animal reaches adulthood, but instead continues throughout
its life. Thus stated, this theory has enough supporting evidence that
it is now widely accepted by the scientific community. As mentioned above, however,
the same cannot be said for the question of neurogenesis in the neocortex of primates.
In this regard, the data are still too contradictory to extend the new theory
to this part of the brain. Further experiments will be needed to tip the balance
in one direction or the other. The moral of this story is that when you
see sensational headlines trumpeting some scientific finding as a sudden lucky
discovery or the product of one scientific genius who has taken the world by storm,
you should always be skeptical. As the above chronology indicates, Elizabeth Gould’s
research, for example, was only the latest in a long series of experiments dating
back to the 1960s, or even back to early 20th century, when Ramon y Cajal conducted
the first experiments in staining neurons. From an even broader perspective, her
research is part of all the scientific efforts that have enabled us to begin to
get a better understanding of the functioning of the human brain.
|