Experiment Module: What Split Brains Tell
Us About Language Communication between the two hemispheres
of the brain is made possible by the bundles of axons, or commissures, that connect
them. The largest of these bundles, known as the corpus callosum, consists of
about 200 million axons running from one hemisphere to the other. In the
1950s, American neuroscientist Roger Sperry and his team discovered that curiously
enough, severing the corpus callosum in the brain of a cat or monkey had no notable
effects on the animal’s behaviour. Only some special experimental protocols
revealed that these animals were actually sometimes behaving as if they had two
brains. This absence of major deficits in animals with a severed corpus
callosum gave neurosurgeons the idea of performing this operation on certain patients
whose frequent, severe epileptic attacks were ruining their lives. In some of
these patients, the epileptic focus was located in only one hemisphere, so this
operation could successfully prevent the attacks from propagating to the other
hemisphere. Having had this operation, these “split-brain” individuals
could go back to enjoying their lives; as with the animals in Sperry’s experiments,
their day-to-day behaviour was practically unaffected by the separation of their
brains into two halves.
The renowned American neuropsychologist Michael Gazzaniga, who began his career
working with Roger Sperry, has developed several devices for analyzing functional
differences between the two hemispheres in split-brain patients. The idea behind
these devices is to deliver stimuli in such a way that they reach only one hemisphere,
and then to observe how this hemisphere manages to process these stimuli on its
own. To study language, Gazzaniga asked his subjects to focus on a point
at the centre of a screen. He then projected images, words, and phrases onto the
screen, to the left or right of this point. By flashing these items quickly enough
that the subjects’ eyes had no time to move, Gazzaniga was able to “talk”
to just one of the hemispheres at a time. Information projected in the subjects’
left visual field was received by the right hemisphere, while information projected
in the right visual field was received by the left. |  |
The subjects could easily repeat numbers or words or describe images
projected in their right visual field, because the left hemisphere, which received
and processed this information, is the dominant hemisphere for language. Similarly,
when asked to close their eyes and feel an object with their right hand, they
could describe the object readily. But when the visual stimuli were projected
in the subjects’ left visual field or when they were asked to feel objects
with their left hand, their performance was quite different: they could not describe
the stimuli or objects concerned. In fact, for the visual stimuli, they even said
that they hadn’t seen anything at all! Though the right hemisphere
does have some serious gaps in its language-processing abilities, it is not completely
devoid of them. It can read and understand numbers, letters, and short statements,
so long as the individual does not have to demonstrate this understanding verbally.
For
example, if the name of an object is projected so that a subject with a severed
corpus callosum sees it with the right hemisphere only, he will say that he doesn’t
see anything, because the severed connection has in fact prevented his left hemisphere,
which is dominant for language, from doing so. But if the experimenter then asks
the subject to use his left hand to choose a card with a drawing of the object
whose name he saw, or to identify this object by feeling it with his left hand,
he will have no problem in performing the task. Thus the right hemisphere cannot
express itself in complex sentences, but it clearly can recognize words. |  |
In another experiment, a photo of a naked man was presented to the
right hemisphere of a female split-brain patient. When asked about the nature
of the photo, she began to laugh and explained that she didn’t know why
she was laughing, but that maybe it was because of the machine that was projecting
the images. Certain
experiments that Gazzaniga conducted with split-brain patients also led him to
develop the concept of the “left-hemisphere interpreter”. In one of
these classic experiments, the split-brain patient had to point with his two hands
at pictures of two objects corresponding to two images that he had seen on the
divided screen (one with each of his two separated hemispheres). In the test shown
here, the patient’s left hand is pointing at the card with a picture of
a snow shovel, because the right hemisphere, which controls this hand, has seen
the projected image of a winter scene. Meanwhile, his right hand is pointing at
the card with a picture of a chicken, because his left hemisphere has seen the
image of a chicken’s foot. |  |
But when the patient is asked to explain why his left hand
is pointing at the shovel, his talking hemisphere—the left one—has
no access to the information seen by the right, and so instead interprets his
behaviour by responding that the reason is that you use a shovel to clean out
the chicken house! Experiments like this show just how ready the brain is to provide
language-based explanations for behaviour. Gazzaniga’s experiments
thus helped to demonstrate the lateralization of language as well as other functional
differences between the left and right hemispheres. 
|